What is the average age of widows?

What is the average age of widows?

LFG Marketing | June 2019

Something you probably don’t know: June 23rd is International Widows Day on the United Nations’ calendar. First observed in 2010, the day was established to raise global awareness of the hardships facing widows following the death of their spouses.

These hardships vary by country and culture. In some so-called “traditional” societies, a woman whose husband has died may be denied an inheritance, evicted from her home, or socially ostracized. In the United States, widows – both men and women – may not encounter the threats to their well-being, but the challenges – emotional and material – can still be significant.

Yet while widowhood is an almost certain event in a marriage, couples often neglect to prepare for it.

Widowhood: Some Statistics

The US Census provides some eye-opening insight into widowhood in the United States. (Note: In US data,

“widowhood” is a gender-neutral term for an individual whose marriage has ended because of the death of a spouse.)

  • It occurs sooner than you might think. In 2016, 24 percent of Americans over the age of 65 had experienced the death of a spouse. Another often-quoted statistic culled from census reports: the median age that widowhood occurs is 59.4.
  • In the United States, widowhood is predominantly a woman’s experience. American women have longer life expectancies and, until recently, have tended to marry men slightly older than themselves. This combination explains why an overwhelming majority of widows – about 85 percent – are women. Women are also more likely to remain widowed as opposed to remarrying.
  • Widowhood often has a negative impact on the finances of a surviving spouse. The Social Security Administration reports that the rate of poverty among elderly widows is three to four times higher than their married counterparts of the same age. Even widows that aren’t impoverished often face major financial adjustments, such as decreased income, loss of health insurance, or the need to relocate.

Not Fun, but Fundamental

Preparing for widowhood is not a “fun” financial task. But most of the preparation is fundamental financial management that ought to be done anyway.

Identify, update and exchange financial information. It’s surprising how uninformed spouses can be about each other’s financial lives. Even in marriages that consciously attempt to integrate finances (joint checking accounts, both names on the mortgage), a lot of financial activity is specific to one spouse; a credit card, a retirement account, an ownership interest in a business, an investment account for dabbling in the stock market, a car with only one name on the title. Both parties should know about the pieces of their financial lives that are separate.

Assemble a comprehensive list of all financial assets, along with statements, passwords, ownership arrangements, and designated beneficiaries, and know where it will be kept. (Secure on-line digital vaults can be a great place to store this information – as long as someone remembers, or can retrieve, the password.)

Plan for succession. One of the advantages of marriage is the division of labor; one person shops, the other does the dishes. And quite often, one person manages the money. If the money-managing spouses dies, can the widow easily assume those duties, or will he/she need help? For older widows, an adult child often serves as a financial assistant, which may be fine. But deciding on a trusted financial professional to serve as a backup is usually a prudent option. Whatever you decide, the key is having someone in place to help manage your affairs.

Review your life insurance. Following the mainstream financial wisdom of their time, many couples bought term life insurance several decades ago, hoping that when the term expired, they would no longer need or want life insurance. But times change. Today, some of these couples recognize that a life insurance benefit would make widowhood much more manageable. If your widowhood scenarios don’t have life insurance, it’s worth looking at how you might get some. If you have life insurance, be sure you can keep it in force until death.

Please, please, please…prepare a will. So many people, even those with lots of assets, never get around to executing a will and trust. We get it; paying for a legal document regarding your future death isn’t fun. But dying without a will makes it harder for individually-owned assets to be transferred or liquidated.

After the death of a spouse, go slow on major decisions. An April 19, 2019, Wall Street Journal article, “Surviving Solo,” notes that the decisions that come in the aftermath of the death of a spouse can be daunting. This is particularly true of real estate because of the “emotional attachments, and in some cases, the needs of the couple’s children.”

To mitigate against impulsive behavior that may be triggered by the passing of a spouse, financial professionals often recommend that widows refrain from making major financial decisions for anywhere from six months to three years. Besides decisions about real estate, this caution applies to other things, like the receipt of life insurance proceeds, and adjustments to inheritance distributions, especially if the widow remarries.

What is the average age of widows?

 Lifetime Financial Growth, LLC is an Agency of The Guardian Life Insurance Company of America® (Guardian), New York, NY. Securities products and advisory services offered through Park Avenue Securities LLC (PAS), member FINRA, SIPC. OSJ: 244 Blvd of the Allies, Pittsburgh, PA 15222 (412) 391-6700. PAS is an indirect, wholly-owned subsidiary of Guardian. This firm is not an affiliate or subsidiary of PAS. 2019-80670 EXP 05/2021

Research on widowhood mostly emphasizes negative outcomes, but some studies suggest widows are resilient. This study evaluated the in-depth daily experiences of older widows compared to married women. Participants included 75 widows and 125 married women from the National Study of Daily Experiences II who completed daily diaries over eight days. For long-term widows, there were no differences compared to married women in daily and general well-being, and a few differences in time use. Results add to the literature by using daily diary data to illustrate the general resilience of older women many years after the death of a spouse.

Keywords: Widowhood, Time use, Daily affect

Widowhood is common among older women; 52% of women 75–84 years of age and 76.2% over age 85 are widows (Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics, 2008). Additionally, over 50% of women over age 70 have ever been widowed (Krieder, 2005). There is a common gender-related stereotype of elderly widows as being frail, vulnerable, and requiring extensive assistance from others (Utz, Reidy, Carr, Nesse, & Wortman, 2004). This “frail-old-woman” stereotype may actually lead others to consider older widows as more dependent and vulnerable than they really are (Utz et al., 2004).

Widowhood does provide a number of challenges, both emotionally and in terms of role changes. The Dual Process Model suggests bereavement requires both loss- and restoration-oriented coping for successful adjustment (Stroebe & Schut, 1999; Utz, 2006). The emotional changes women experience during bereavement require loss-oriented coping, while the inevitable changes in daily life necessitate restoration-oriented coping.

Most research on widows focuses on declines in well-being (loss-oriented coping) after the death of a spouse. These studies associate widowhood with lower morale and social engagement (Bennett, 2005), increased anxiety or anger if the spouse was in physical pain prior to death (Carr, 2003), increased depressive symptoms (Fry, 2001; Lee, deMaris, Bavin, & Sullivan, 2001), and clinically significant symptoms a year later for an estimated 15% of recently bereaved widows (Bonanno & Kaltman, 2001). Contrary to this emphasis on problems during widowhood, Bonanno and colleagues’ evaluation of widowhood trajectories demonstrates that the highest percentage (45.9%) of widows in their study exhibited resilient characteristics with low depressive symptoms during the 18 months after the death of their spouse (Bonanno et al., 2002; Bonanno, 2009). Some widows may actually show improvements in well-being after the death of a spouse, which may be associated with declines in strains such as caregiving (Bonanno et al., 2002; Schulz et al., 2003). Based on this research, it is not well understood to what extent psychological resilience might characterize women beyond the first couple years of widowhood. Additionally, widows who were, on average, 12.6 years post loss reported that although they thought about their spouse once every week or so, they only “sometimes” or “rarely” were upset by these thoughts (Carnelley, Wortman, Bolger, & Burke, 2006). Qualitative research comparing the short and long-term effects of widowhood has begun to indicate that the initial short-term negative effects may be accompanied by longer-term positive effects, or resiliency (Feldman, Byles, & Beaumont, 2000). Recent widows (less than one year) are more likely to have worse social functioning and depressed mood compared to women who have been widowed for more than a year (Wilcox et al., 2003). Preliminary research evaluating loss-oriented coping suggests widowhood may be more benign over the long term than is typically found in studies assessing distress very recently after the death of a spouse, but research is limited beyond the first few years post loss.

Research on ways that older women who are widows develop new activities and roles after loss (restoration-oriented coping) is less developed (Carr & Utz, 2001; Utz, 2006; Utz, Reidy et al., 2004). However, psychological interventions for depression and grief often include an emphasis on altering daily activities (Qualls & Knight, 2008; Worden, 2008) and some researchers suggest that the daily life changes may also be an important area for research in widowhood (Feldman et al., 2000). Past studies suggest that widows may decrease time doing chores after their spouse dies (Utz et al., 2004). Shortly after a spouse’s death, a bereaved partner may spend more time receiving support and less time giving support to their children (Ha, Carr, Utz, & Nesse, 2006). Widows may require more assistance in daily activities, whereas married women may have more resources available to provide support to their children. Utz and colleagues found evidence suggesting that widows and non-widows had similar levels of “formal social participation,” but that widows spent more time in “informal social participation” than did married women (Utz, Carr, Nesse & Wortman, 2002). The authors included volunteering, organizational meetings, and religious services together as “formal social participation” and getting together or talking on the phone with friends/relatives as “informal social participation” (Utz et al., 2002). Informal social participation could also encompass other activities, such as exchanging social support or spending time with children. Other individuals in addition to children, such as friends, neighbors, or religious groups, may also provide assistance to widows, and research examining these relationships is limited. Past research provides an idea of changes in widows’ time use during adjustment to widowhood using retrospective measures that consider a range of activities describing formal and informal participation (Utz et al., 2002). Although research has begun to understand some of the general changes in time use (e.g., Utz et al., 2004; Utz et al., 2002; Ha, 2001; Ha et al., 2006), further research evaluating the specific day-to-day activities of non-recent widows may provide a better understanding of the long-term well-being of widows.

Daily diary methods evaluate individuals every day for a period of days, allowing researchers to look at well-being through what is described as the “microscope” of social science as an alternative to traditional retrospective measures that may risk recall bias (Almeida, 2005; Bolger, Davis, & Rafaeli, 2003). The current study hopes to better understand the specific daily activities in widowhood by comparing daily time use (Aim 1) and both daily and global well-being (Aim 2) in widowed and married women. In our comparison of widows and married womens’ daily time use, we hypothesized that widows would spend less time doing chores, more time with children, and more time receiving support than married women. Although research is lacking in the comparison of daily well-being for widows and married women, Bonanno’s (2009) research led us to hypothesize that this sample of non-recent widowed women would exhibit resilience with similar levels of daily and global well-being compared to married women.

Data were from the second wave (2004–2006) of the National Study of Daily Experiences (NSDE II), a satellite study of the second wave of the Midlife in the United States Survey (MIDUS II; for descriptions of the NSDE and MIDUS project, see Almeida, Wethington, & Kessler, 2002: Mroczek & Kolarz, 1998). NSDE II participants were randomly selected from the larger MIDUS II study, and the response rate for the NSDE II sample was 86%. Respondents in this study included widows and married women, aged 60 and older, from the initial sample of 2,022 adults who had participated in NSDE II.

Demographic variables included in the analyses were age (measured continuously), education (percent with high school degree), number of children (measured continuously), race (White or non-White), and employment status (working or not working). Seventy-five widows and a demographically-similar comparison group of 125 married women were included in the analyses. Time since widowhood was evaluated among the widows, measured continuously as the number of years since the spouse’s death.

The study evaluated demographic information for the two groups, including age, education, number of children, race, time since widowhood (for widows), and employment status (Table 1). Widows were slightly older than married women, and the other demographic variables were not significantly different between the two groups. Most of the married and widowed women were White and not working. Widows were not recently bereaved.

Demographic Information of Widowed (n = 75) and Married Women (n = 125)

WidowsMarried Woment or χ2
M (SD) or %M (SD) or %
Age72.37 (6.67)70.03 (5.97)−2.57*
Percent with high school degree82.691.23.23
Number of children3.48 (1.90)3.39 (1.80)−.33
Percent with no children1.31.60.02
Race (percent White)87.895.2−1.72
Years widowed11.12 (8.26)n/an/a
Percent working21.316.00.98
 Full-time (of percent working)31.86.12.33

Respondents participated in eight consecutive days of daily telephone interviews where they reported on their daily time use and daily well-being. In addition to these daily interviews, respondents also completed self-report questionnaires as part of the larger MIDUS II study. Demographic information and global well-being were assessed as part of MIDUS II. Global well-being was measured by asking respondents, “In general, would you say your mental/emotional health is excellent, very good, good, fair or poor?” Global well-being was measured with a 5-point Likert-type scale that was reverse-coded, ranging from 1 (poor) to 5 (excellent; Idler & Kasl, 1991).

Daily time use

Each day, respondents were asked how much time they spent with children, doing housework, doing work or schoolwork, relaxing or doing leisure activities, watching television, volunteering, giving or receiving unpaid assistance, giving or receiving emotional support, providing assistance to someone with a disability, and sleeping. To gain a better understanding of the nature and meaning of the exchange of support in widows and married women, we also report the relationship of the person who is either providing support/assistance or receiving support/assistance with the older women in our sample. Daily time use was measured for eight consecutive days, and aggregate measures of each type of time use were used in the current analyses (McDonald & Almeida, 2004).

Daily measures of well-being

Daily positive and negative affect were measured using an adapted inventory of emotions from the Non-Specific Psychological Distress Scale developed from other validated scales for use in MIDUS I (Mroczek & Kolarz, 1998). In NSDE II, the psychological distress scale was supplemented with the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark & Tellegen, 1988) and calibrated for daily affect. Daily negative affect items included how often during the past day they experienced fourteen different negative emotions (e.g. worthless, hopeless, and restless or fidgety), and daily positive affect items included how often during the past day they experienced thirteen different positive emotions (e.g., in good spirits, cheerful, and calm and peaceful). Respondents rated their responses on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 (none of the time) to 4 (all of the time), and greater scores reflected higher levels of positive and negative affect. Scores across the positive and negative affect items were summed and averaged across the interview week to produce two scores for analysis. Cronbach’s alpha was .91 for negative affect and .96 for positive affect.

Widows who remarried were not included in the sample, and 125 married women were selected using a dataset with demographic characteristics (age, race, highest level of education achieved, number of children, employment status) to create a similar comparison group. For the two research aims, analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), controlling for age, was run by marital status on all daily time use variables in one model, and on daily and global well-being measures in a second model. Independent samples t-tests were run comparing the relationship of the person involved in the exchange or either emotional support or instrumental assistance between the two groups, married and widowed women.

Before conducting analyses comparing widows and married women, initial tests were run to evaluate any differences between recent and non-recent widows within the widows sample. The small group of widows in the sample with less than two years since their spouse’s death (n = 18) were generally similar to the non-recent widows (n = 57). However, recent widows were slightly younger (M = 69.39, SD = 5.95 years vs. M = 73.30, SD = 6.70 years), t(72) = 2.21, p < .05. Controlling for the age difference between non-recent and recent widows, there were no differences in daily time use, daily affect, or global well-being, so results were pooled for all widows in subsequent analyses.

ANCOVA analyses revealed similarities in most of the daily time use measures (Table 2). Both widows and married women spent two hours doing chores and less than an hour doing work or school activities. On average, widows and married women spent over three hours participating in leisure activities and approximately 30 minutes engaging in vigorous activity. Widows and married women spent approximately one hour volunteering, almost two hours giving unpaid assistance, and 15–20 minutes receiving assistance. Widows and married women spent almost an hour giving emotional support, 30–40 minutes receiving emotional support, and from 45 minutes to an hour assisting someone with a disability. Consistent with our hypothesis, widows spent significantly more time with children than married women. Widows spent significantly less time sleeping and more time watching TV than married women.

Analysis of Covariance for Time Use Variables in Widows (n = 75) and Married Women (n = 125)a

WidowsMarried WomenF
M (SD) or %M (SD) or %
Time Use Variable
 With Children0.93 (1.67)0.55 (1.19)3.97*
 Doing Chores1.68 (1.06)1.99 (1.17)2.71
 Doing Work/School Activities0.77 (1.57)0.65 (1.43)1.10
 Leisure3.21 (1.80)3.08 (1.70)0.54
 Watching TV2.51 (1.41)2.05 (1.28)6.20*
 Engaging in Vigorous Activity0.52 (.61)0.60 (.67)0.47
 Volunteering1.04 (1.65)0.83 (1.35)1.55
 Giving Unpaid Assistance1.66 (3.18)2.03 (3.24)0.24
 Receiving Unpaid Assistance0.37 (1.32)0.17 (.85)0.75
 Giving Support0.96 (1.42)0.88 (1.07)0.30
 Receiving Support0.56 (.79)0.69 (2.22)0.21
 Assisting Disabled1.09 (2.89)0.80 (1.54)1.35
 Sleeping6.91 (1.07)7.22 (.94)4.71*

Exchanges of emotional support

Although total time spent giving and total time spent receiving support was similar for married women and widows, results of independent samples t-tests revealed some differences in the person involved in the exchange of emotional support between the two groups (see Table 3). Widows both gave and received significantly more daily emotional support from friends/neighbors than did married women. Widows also received significantly more daily emotional support from religious groups than did married women in our sample. While married women in our sample received the highest amount of daily emotional support from spouses, widows, in contrast, received equally high amounts of daily emotional support from both children and friends/neighbors. In sum, while married women receive most of their emotional support from their spouses, widows may have supplemented their need for support from other sources (children, friends, neighbors, and religious groups).

Independent Samples T-test of Support and Assistance Exchanged in Widows (n = 75) and Married Women (n = 125)a

Widows (%)Married Women (%)
Giving emotional support
 Spouse0.00***14.93
 Child19.4524.70
 Sibling/Other Relative19.9821.95
 Friend/neighbor54.68***34.15
 Religious Group3.112.41
 Other12.4610.32
Receiving emotional support
 Spouse1.0438.76
 Child30.0119.68
 Sibling18.3912.11
 Friend/neighbor40.93*25.20
 Religious Group12.75*3.24
 Other6.5512.95
Giving unpaid assistance
 Spouse0.000.00
 Child44.3547.68
 Sibling/Other Relative25.7021.69
 Friend/neighbor39.49*22.16
 Religious Group5.692.36
 Other4.22*11.79
Receiving unpaid assistance
 Spouse0.006.23
 Child24.99*3.13
 Sibling/Other Relative4.1618.75
 Friend/neighbor9.3812.47
 Religious Group0.006.25
 Other36.45*6.25

Exchanges of instrumental support

With regard to giving and receiving daily instrumental assistance, similar patterns emerged in independent samples t-test results between married women and widows. Widows gave significantly more assistance to their friends/neighbors than did married women, and widows received more daily assistance from children and others (strangers, non-profit groups, etc.) than did married women. Overall, married women gave the most instrumental assistance to their children. Widows, however, both gave and received comparable amounts of instrumental assistance with friends, neighbors, siblings, and other relatives in addition to their children.

As anticipated, there were no statistically significant differences in either daily or global well-being between the two groups. Widows and married women, on average, reported experiencing daily negative affect “none” or “a little of the time” (M = 0.15, SD = 0.16 vs. M = 0.14, SD = 0.15, respectively) and they reported experiencing daily positive emotions “most of the time” (M = 2.93, SD = 0.60 vs. M = 2.89, SD = 0.57, respectively). Global measures of well-being suggest that widows and married women self-report their mental/emotional health as either “average” or “good” (M = 3.77, SD = 0.91 vs. M = 3.78, SD = 0.85).

This study sought to evaluate potential differences and similarities in daily time use and both daily and global well-being between widows and married women. Overall, we found little evidence to support the stereotype of older women who are long-term widows as frail and dependent, and considerable evidence that these widows were psychologically healthy and functioning similarly to women of similar age who were still married. Widows and married women showed no differences in daily or general well being, and similarities in most aspects of time use. Differences that were found were small in magnitude, but of interest in understanding how widowhood affects daily life. Widows spent more time watching TV than married females, which may be reflective of increased solitary activities as a result of living alone. The difference in time spent sleeping (widows sleeping less, on average) might be related to increased TV time. Controlling for age, widows spent significantly more time with children, suggesting that widows continue to appreciate companionship from children over time. We predicted that widows would spend more time doing chores and receive more support than married women; however, these hypotheses were not supported. Because these widows have had time to cope with both the loss-and restoration-oriented aspects of widowhood, similarities in time use and well-being may be indicative of resilience over many years of adjustment. Consistent with Bonnano et al.’s research, most women may return to normal functioning after the death of a spouse (Bonnano et al., 2002; Bonanno, 2009), and widows who live on average 11 years after bereavement may be generally healthy and return to pre-bereavement daily activities.

The differences in the exchange of daily emotional support and unpaid daily assistance may reflect adaptation in both loss- and restoration-oriented coping. Widows exchange more daily emotional support with their friends, neighbors, siblings, other relatives, and children; this exchange in multiple relationships may help women cope with the loss of the close relationship they may have had with their spouse (loss-oriented coping). Conversely, the increased unpaid daily assistance that widows receive from non-spousal relationships may reflect the widows’ need for increased support to cope with changes in daily life post loss (restoration-oriented coping).

As predicted, neither daily nor global well-being were significantly different between the two groups. The current study adds information regarding daily well-being to the current field consisting primarily of global well-being measures and retrospective studies. This research supports findings that non-recent widows as a whole may be doing well, and adds to this research with similar results regarding daily assessments of time use and well-being. Despite the large number of studies identifying potential declines related to well-being (Bennett, 2005; Fry, 2001; Lee et al., 2001; Carr, 2003), daily aspects of widowhood may be generally less negative than previously thought.

Overall, results refute stereotypes of widows as frail, dependent, and distressed, and support recent research showing that most widows do not have clinically significant distress with the passage of sufficient time. Results indicate that many widows are resilient and find ways to accommodate to the loss of a spouse through day-to-day activities. In working with older women, it is important to acknowledge both the vulnerabilities that widowhood can provide, but also to recognize the strengths that many older women have in facing this very common late life transition. Specifically, assistance from friends or neighbors (e.g., yard work, cooking) may help widows adjust to the changes in daily life that may become stressful over time.

  • Almeida DM. Resilience and vulnerability to daily stressors assessed via diary methods. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2005;14:64–68. [Google Scholar]
  • Almeida DM, Wethington E, Kessler RC. The daily inventory of stressful events: An interview-based approach for measuring daily stressors. Assessment. 2002;9:41–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bennett KM. Psychological wellbeing in later life: The longitudinal effects of marriage, widowhood and marital status change. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2005;20:280–284. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bolger N, Davis A, Rafaeli E. Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology. 2003;54:579–616. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bonanno GA. The other side of sadness: What the new science of bereavement tells us about life after loss. New York: Basic Books; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • Bonanno GA, Kaltman S. The varieties of grief experience. Clinical Psychology Review. 2001;21:705–734. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bonanno GA, Wortman CB, Lehman DR, Tweed RG, Haring M, Sonnega J, et al. Resilience to loss and chronic grief: A prospective study from preloss to 18-months postloss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002;83:1150–1164. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Carnelley KB, Wortman CB, Bolger N, Burke CT. The time course of grief reactions to spousal loss: Evidence from a national probability sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;91:476–492. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Carr D. A “good death” for whom? Quality of spouse’s death and psychological distress among older widowed persons. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2003;44:215–232. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Carr D, Utz R. Late-life widowhood in the United States: New directions in research and theory. Ageing International. 2001;27:65–88. [Google Scholar]
  • Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics. Older Americans 2008: Key indicators of well-being. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • Feldman S, Byles JE, Beaumont R. ‘Is anybody listening?’ The experiences of widowhood for older Australian women. Journal of Women & Aging. 2000;12:155–176. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fry PS. Predictors of health-related quality of life perspectives, self-esteem, and life satisfactions of older adults following spousal loss: An 18-month follow-up study of widows and widowers. The Gerontologist. 2001;41:787–798. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ha J. Unpublished manuscript. University of Michigan; 2001. Gender differences in the dynamics of intergenerational dependence after widowhood: An examination of specific domains of support. [Google Scholar]
  • Ha J, Carr D, Utz RL, Nesse R. Older adults’ perceptions of intergenerational support after widowhood: How do men and women differ? Journal of Family Issues. 2006;27:3–30. [Google Scholar]
  • Idler EL, Kasl S. Health perceptions and survival: Do global evaluations of health status really predict mortality? Journals of Gerontology. 1991;46:S55–S65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Krieder RM. Current Population Reports. Washington, D.C: U.S. Census Bureau; 2005. Number, timing, and duration of marriages and divorces: 2001; pp. P70–97. [Google Scholar]
  • Lee GR, deMaris A, Bavin S, Sullivan R. Gender differences in the depressive effect of widowhood in later life. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences. 2001;56B:S56–S61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • McDonald DA, Almeida DM. The interweave of father’s daily work experiences and fathering behaviors. Fathering. 2004;2:235–251. [Google Scholar]
  • Mroczek DK, Kolarz CM. The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;75:1333–1349. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Qualls SH, Knight BG, editors. Psychotherapy for depression in older adults. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • Schulz R, Mendelsohn AB, Haley WE, Mahoney D, Allen RS, Zhang S, et al. End of life care and the effects of bereavement on family caregivers of persons with dementia. New England Journal of Medicine. 2003;349:1936–1942. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Stroebe M, Schut H. The dual process model of coping with bereavement: Rationale and description. Death Studies. 1999;23:197–224. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Utz R. Economic and practical adjustments to late life spousal loss. In: Carr D, Nesse RM, Wortman CB, editors. Spousal Bereavement in Late Life. New York: Springer; 2006. pp. 167–192. [Google Scholar]
  • Utz RL, Carr D, Nesse R, Wortman CB. The effect of widowhood on older adults’ social participation: An evaluation of activity, disengagement, and continuity theories. The Gerontologist. 2002;42:522–533. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Utz RL, Reidy EB, Carr D, Nesse R, Wortman C. The daily consequences of widowhood: The role of gender and intergenerational transfers on subsequent housework performance. Journal of Family Issues. 2004;25:683–712. [Google Scholar]
  • Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54:1063–1070. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wilcox S, Evenson KR, Aragaki A, Wassertheil-Smoller S, Mouton CP, Loevinger BL. The effects of widowhood on physical and mental health, health behaviors, and health outcomes: The Women’s Health Initiative. Health Psychology. 2003;22:513–522. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Worden JW. Grief counseling and grief therapy: A handbook for the mental health practitioner. New York: Springer; 2008. [Google Scholar]